journal
https://read.qxmd.com/read/32952462/reconsidering-retrieval-effects-on-adult-regularization-of-inconsistent-variation-in-language
#21
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Carla L Hudson Kam
The phenomenon of regularization - learners imposing systematicity on inconsistent variation in language input - is complex. Studies show that children are more likely to regularize than adults, but adults will also regularize under certain circumstances. Exactly why we see the pattern of behaviour that we do is not well understood, however. This paper reports on an experiment investigating whether it is possible to induce regularization in adults by varying the conditions of learning and/or testing in ways that made retrieval more difficult, something predicted by Hudson Kam and Newport (2009)...
2019: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/31607832/learning-phonology-from-surface-distributions-considering-dutch-and-english-vowel-duration
#22
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Daniel Swingley
In learning language, children must discover how to interpret the linguistic significance of phonetic variation. On some accounts, receptive phonology is grounded in perceptual learning of phonetic categories from phonetic distributions drawn over the infant's sample of speech. On other accounts, receptive phonology is instead based on phonetic generalizations over the words in the lexicon. Tests of these hypotheses have been rare and indirect, usually making use of idealized estimates of phonetic variation...
2019: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/31602179/look-at-mommy-an-exploratory-study-of-attention-related-communication-in-mothers-of-toddlers-at-risk-for-autism
#23
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Karen P Jakubowski, Jana M Iverson
Attentional difficulties are evident in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Accordingly, mothers of children with ASD may modify communication to direct their child's attention, and this pattern may generalize to later-born children. This study examined patterns of child-directed communication in eleven mothers of 18-month-old toddlers at heightened risk (HR) for ASD and compared them to eleven low-risk (LR; no first- or second-degree relative with ASD) dyads. Naturalistic interactions at home were coded for communication that captured, directed, or maintained children's attention and/or actions...
2019: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/32952461/visual-sonority-modulates-infants-attraction-to-sign-language
#24
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Adam Stone, Laura-Ann Petitto, Rain Bosworth
The infant brain may be predisposed to identify perceptually salient cues that are common to both signed and spoken languages. Recent theory based on spoken languages has advanced sonority as one of these potential language acquisition cues. Using a preferential looking paradigm with an infrared eye tracker, we explored visual attention of hearing 6- and 12-month-olds with no sign language experience as they watched fingerspelling stimuli that either conformed to high sonority (well-formed) or low sonority (ill-formed) values, which are relevant to syllabic structure in signed language...
2018: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/30774569/processing-and-comprehension-of-accented-speech-by-monolingual-and-bilingual-children
#25
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Margarethe McDonald, Megan Gross, Milijana Buac, Michelle Batko, Margarita Kaushanskaya
This study tested the effect of Spanish-accented speech on sentence comprehension in children with different degrees of Spanish experience. The hypothesis was that earlier acquisition of Spanish would be associated with enhanced comprehension of Spanish-accented speech. Three groups of 5-6 year old children were tested: monolingual English-speaking children, simultaneous Spanish-English bilingual children and early English-Spanish bilingual children. The children completed a semantic judgment task in English on semantically meaningful and nonsensical sentences produced by a native English speaker and a native Spanish speaker characterized by a strong Spanish accent...
2018: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/30416398/when-veps-cry-two-year-olds-efficiently-learn-novel-words-from-linguistic-contexts-alone
#26
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Brock Ferguson, Eileen Graf, Sandra R Waxman
We assessed 24-month-old infants' lexical processing efficiency for both novel and familiar words. Prior work documented that 19-month-olds successfully identify referents of familiar words (e.g., The dog is so little ) as well as novel words whose meanings were informed only by the surrounding sentence (e.g., The vep is crying ), but that the speed with which they identify the referents of novel words lagged far behind that for familiar words. Here we take a developmental approach, extending this work to 24-month-olds...
2018: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/30899202/primitive-concepts-of-number-and-the-developing-human-brain
#27
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Alyssa J Kersey, Jessica F Cantlon
Counting is an evolutionarily recent cultural invention of the human species. In order for humans to have conceived of counting in the first place, certain representational and logical abilities must have already been in place. The focus of this review is the origins and nature of those fundamental mechanisms that promoted the emergence of the human number concept. Five claims are presented that support an evolutionary view of numerical development: 1) number is an abstract concept with an innate basis in humans, 2) maturational processes constrain the development of humans' numerical representations between infancy and adulthood, 3) there is evolutionary continuity in the neural processes of numerical cognition in primates, 4) primitive logical abilities support verbal counting development in humans, and 5) primitive neural processes provide the foundation for symbolic numerical development in the human brain...
2017: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/30740037/2-5-year-olds-retention-and-generalization-of-novel-words-across-short-and-long-delays
#28
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Erica H Wojcik
Two experiments investigated two-year-olds' retention and generalization of novel words across short and long time delays. Specifically, retention of newly learned words and generalization to novel exemplars or novel contexts were tested one minute or one week after learning. Experiment 1 revealed successful retention as well as successful generalization to both new exemplars and new contexts after a one-minute delay, with no statistical differences between retention and generalization performance for either generalization type...
2017: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/30405323/the-effect-of-zipfian-frequency-variations-on-category-formation-in-adult-artificial-language-learning
#29
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Kathryn D Schuler, Patricia A Reeder, Elissa L Newport, Richard N Aslin
Successful language acquisition hinges on organizing individual words into grammatical categories and learning the relationships between them, but the method by which children accomplish this task has been debated in the literature. One proposal is that learners use the shared distributional contexts in which words appear as a cue to their underlying category structure. Indeed, recent research using artificial languages has demonstrated that learners can acquire grammatical categories from this type of distributional information...
2017: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/29200981/semantic-specificity-in-one-year-olds-word-comprehension
#30
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Elika Bergelson, Richard Aslin
The present study investigated infants' knowledge about familiar nouns. Infants (n = 46, 12-20-month-olds) saw two-image displays of familiar objects, or one familiar and one novel object. Infants heard either a matching word (e.g. "foot' when seeing foot and juice), a related word (e.g. "sock" when seeing foot and juice) or a nonce word (e.g. "fep" when seeing a novel object and dog). Across the whole sample, infants reliably fixated the referent on matching and nonce trials. On the critical related trials we found increasingly less looking to the incorrect (but related) image with age...
2017: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/28983210/the-development-of-causal-structure-without-a-language-model
#31
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Lilia Rissman, Susan Goldin-Meadow
Across a diverse range of languages, children proceed through similar stages in their production of causal language: their initial verbs lack internal causal structure, followed by a period during which they produce causative overgeneralizations, indicating knowledge of a productive causative rule. We asked in this study whether a child not exposed to structured linguistic input could create linguistic devices for encoding causation and, if so, whether the emergence of this causal language would follow a trajectory similar to the one observed for children learning language from linguistic input...
2017: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/28966565/are-homophones-acoustically-distinguished-in-child-directed-speech
#32
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Erin Conwell
Many approaches to early word learning posit that children assume a one-to-one mapping of form and meaning. However, children's early vocabularies contain homophones, words that violate that assumption. Children might learn such words by exploiting prosodic differences between homophone meanings that are associated with lemma frequency (Gahl, 2008). Such differences have not yet been documented in children's natural language experience and the exaggerated prosody of child-directed speech could either mask the subtle distinctions reported in adult-directed speech or enhance them...
2017: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/28496393/subtlety-of-ambient-language-effects-in-babbling-a-study-of-english-and-chinese-learning-infants-at-8-10-and-12-months
#33
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Chia-Cheng Lee, Yuna Jhang, Li-Mei Chen, George Relyea, D Kimbrough Oller
Prior research on ambient-language effects in babbling has often suggested infants produce language-specific phonological features within the first year. These results have been questioned in research failing to find such effects and challenging the positive findings on methodological grounds. We studied English- and Chinese-learning infants at 8, 10, and 12 months and found listeners could not detect ambient-language effects in the vast majority of infant utterances, but only in items deemed to be words or to contain canonical syllables that may have made them sound like words with language-specific shapes...
2017: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/28344520/does-the-approximate-number-system-serve-as-a-foundation-for-symbolic-mathematics
#34
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Emily Szkudlarek, Elizabeth M Brannon
No abstract text is available yet for this article.
2017: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/28286431/input-subject-diversity-enhances-early-grammatical-growth-evidence-from-a-parent-implemented-intervention
#35
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Pamela A Hadley, Matthew Rispoli, Janet K Holt, Theodora Papastratakos, Ning Hsu, Mary Kubalanza, Megan M McKenna
PURPOSE: The current study used an intervention design to test the hypothesis that parent input sentences with diverse lexical noun phrase (NP) subjects would accelerate growth in children's sentence diversity. METHOD: Child growth in third person sentence diversity was modeled from 21 to 30 months ( n = 38) in conversational language samples obtained at 21, 24, 27, and 30 months. Treatment parents ( n = 19) received instruction on strategies designed to increase lexical NP subjects (e...
2017: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/27019652/german-children-s-use-of-word-order-and-case-marking-to-interpret-simple-and-complex-sentences-testing-differences-between-constructions-and-lexical-items
#36
Silke Brandt, Elena Lieven, Michael Tomasello
Children and adults follow cues such as case marking and word order in their assignment of semantic roles in simple transitives (e.g., the dog chased the cat). It has been suggested that the same cues are used for the interpretation of complex sentences, such as transitive relative clauses (RCs) (e.g., that's the dog that chased the cat) (Bates, Devescovi, & D'Amico, 1999). We used a pointing paradigm to test German-speaking 3-, 4-, and 6-year-old children's sensitivity to case marking and word order in their interpretation of simple transitives and transitive RCs...
April 2, 2016: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/29527136/noun-and-verb-production-in-maternal-and-child-language-continuity-stability-and-prediction-across-the-second-year-of-life
#37
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Emiddia Longobardi, Pietro Spataro, Diane L Putnick, Marc H Bornstein
The present study examined continuity/discontinuity and stability/instability of noun and verb production measures in 30 child-mother dyads observed at 16 and 20 months, and predictive relations with the acquisition of nouns and verbs at 24 months. Children exhibited significant discontinuity and robust stability in the frequency of nouns and verbs between 16 and 20 months (over and above the contribution of maternal measures). By contrast, mothers showed small, but significant, increases in the total number of nouns and the percentages of nouns located in the initial and final utterance positions, together with a decrease in the percentage of verbs located in the initial position...
2016: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/27672354/two-and-three-year-olds-track-a-single-meaning-during-word-learning-evidence-for-propose-but-verify
#38
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Kristina Woodard, Lila R Gleitman, John C Trueswell
A child word-learning experiment is reported that examines 2- and 3-year-olds' ability to learn the meanings of novel words across multiple, referentially ambiguous, word occurrences. Children were told they were going on an animal safari in which they would learn the names of unfamiliar animals. Critical trial sequences began with hearing a novel word (e.g., "I see a dax! Point to the dax!") while seeing photos of two unfamiliar animals. After responding and performing on two filler trials with known animals, participants encountered the novel word again ("I see another dax! Point to the dax!") in one of two experimental conditions...
2016: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/27284274/the-influence-of-phonotactic-probability-and-neighborhood-density-on-children-s-production-of-newly-learned-words
#39
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Lori Heisler, Lisa Goffman
A word learning paradigm was used to teach children novel words that varied in phonotactic probability and neighborhood density. The effects of frequency and density on speech production were examined when phonetic forms were non-referential (i.e., when no referent was attached) and when phonetic forms were referential (i.e., when a referent was attached through fast mapping). Two methods of analysis were included: (1) kinematic variability of speech movement patterning; and (2) measures of segmental accuracy...
2016: Language Learning and Development
https://read.qxmd.com/read/27103880/does-the-owl-fly-out-of-the-tree-or-does-the-owl-exit-the-tree-flying-how-l2-learners-overcome-their-l1-lexicalization-biases
#40
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Lulu Song, Rachel Pulverman, Christina Pepe, Roberta Michnick Golinkoff, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek
Learning a language is more than learning its vocabulary and grammar. For example, compared to English, Spanish uses many more path verbs such as ascender ('to move upward') and salir ('to go out'), and expresses manner of motion optionally. English, in contrast, has many manner verbs (e.g., run, jog) and expresses path in prepositional phrases (e.g., out of the barn). The way in which a language encodes an event is known as its lexicalization pattern or bias. Using a written sentence elicitation task, we asked whether adult Spanish learners whose L1 was English adopted Spanish lexicalization biases, and what types of L2 exposure facilitated the learning of lexicalization biases...
2016: Language Learning and Development
journal
journal
40923
2
3
Fetch more papers »
Fetching more papers... Fetching...
Remove bar
Read by QxMD icon Read
×

Save your favorite articles in one place with a free QxMD account.

×

Search Tips

Use Boolean operators: AND/OR

diabetic AND foot
diabetes OR diabetic

Exclude a word using the 'minus' sign

Virchow -triad

Use Parentheses

water AND (cup OR glass)

Add an asterisk (*) at end of a word to include word stems

Neuro* will search for Neurology, Neuroscientist, Neurological, and so on

Use quotes to search for an exact phrase

"primary prevention of cancer"
(heart or cardiac or cardio*) AND arrest -"American Heart Association"

We want to hear from doctors like you!

Take a second to answer a survey question.